Stop Circus Suffering

Stop Circus Suffering USA: 4 The Scientific Evidence

Part 4 of ADI’s “Stop Circus Suffering USA” report, discussing the scientific evidence

Bailey Brothers Circus elephant

4.1 Effects of Transport

A scientific review of research on the transportation of horses concluded: “Although some horses adapt to transport much better than others, transport is generally associated with lower reproductive rates, increased disease incidence, a temporary reduction in athletic performance and the alteration of many other physiological traits that are indicative of stress….Transported horses can be subjected to a wide range of potential stressors, including isolation from herd-mates, forced close proximity to unfamiliar or aggressive horses, novel or threatening surroundings, exposure to new pathogens, restraint of normal activity patterns, forced adoption of an abnormal posture, extremes in temperature, water and feed deprivation, and blowing dust and particulate matter. Transport has long been associated with morbidity in horses” 10.

These problems occur across a wide range of species for varied durations and conditions of transport and are a key factor in traveling circus life, a serious welfare problem for animals.

4.1.1 Exotic Species

  • Alpacas and other camelids, who traveled for just 30 minutes, suffered from a resulting potentially dangerous hormone disorder called hypercortisolemia (Cushings syndrome). When the trip ended at a location familiar to the animals it still took four hours for their cortisol hormone levels to return to normal 11. Circus animals are often transported for longer periods and, unlike these alpacas, transported to unfamiliar locations.
  • Tigers in circuses show a wide range of abnormal behaviors and coping strategies while traveling. One of the most common stereotypic behaviors is pacing. In one study pacing increased as travel time increased 6,9.
  • A study of captive black rhinoceroses demonstrated a connection between transport and the immediate development of a skin disease 12.
  • Many species are known to suffer from capture myopathy, a syndrome that occurs in wild mammals and birds, whether free-ranging or captive, associated with the stress of capture, restraint and transportation. In many animals the syndrome is characterized by clinical depression, muscular stiffness, lack of coordination, paralysis, metabolic acidosis and death 13.

4.1.2 Domestic Species

There are numerous studies on the effects of transportation on horses and other animals used in agriculture. This data also applies to animals in traveling circuses.

Horses
Substantial evidence has shown that horses suffer during transportation. Equine expert and veterinary behaviorist Dr. Paul McGreevy discusses transportation of horses 14.

“Horses brace themselves against and in anticipation of the changes in momentum during road transport by adopting certain body postures (notably the base wide stance). Efforts expended by horses as they continually adjust their posture during transit reflect both muscular and emotional stress related to road conditions and the drivers behavior. All of these efforts are readily evaluated by monitoring heart rates during transport. Horses have been shown to have higher heart rates in a moving vehicle than in a stationary vehicle, and although heart rates decreased significantly during a road journey, they did not return to resting levels. Transport stress may increase susceptibility to diseases, including an equine herpes virus and salmonellosis infections”.

  • A study of the effects of a 24-hour transport in horses showed that “Plasma cortisol concentration increased during loading and the first 3 hours of transport and continued to rise throughout the 24 hours, to peak at the termination of transport….after the stressor [i.e., transportation] ceased, cortisol dramatically declined” 15.
  • Many studies show that transport induces weight loss in some animals. Immediately following transit in one study, horses showed a 6 percent weight loss. The investigators thought this could be due to “heat dissipation, sweat loss, and decreased gut fill during transit,” but there was still a 3 percent weight loss 24 hours after the transportation period 15.
  • The immune systems of horses are compromised by the effects of transportation. Stull and Rodiek 15 concluded that this could result in increased susceptibility to infectious diseases. A similar study by Stull et al 16 found elevations in cortisol concentration, white blood cell count, and other physiological changes, which led to the same conclusion. It also acknowledges that “a small window of immunological uncertainty follows long-term transportation, enhancing the potential risk of infectious disease in susceptible individuals.”
  • Aggression among horses increases during transportation, especially when many horses are transported together 17. About 20 percent of all horses receive some type of injury during transportation, most of which occurs to the head and face 18,19. Another paper reports “many incidences of aggressive horses repeatedly biting an adjacent horse in an apparent effort to get the horse to move away” 17.

Cattle
Transport is thought to be one of the most potent stressors for cattle 20. Stressors involved in the transportation of cattle result in altered nutritional status and animal behavior, reduced body weight gain, feed consumption, immune function and increased mortality 21.

  • Long-term transportation in cattle has been found to increase the secretion of ACTH (adrenocorticotropin hormone), a regulator of immune responsiveness, and therefore to exert a negative effect on the immune system. When cattle are rested and fed on their truck when it is stationary, their ACTH levels remain high, coming down only when they are removed from their truck and rested and fed in stalls for 24 hours 22.
  • Transport stress induces an increase in thyroid and adrenal activity in cattle that is evident even after a short trip and continues to increase after long-distance transport 20.
  • Long-term transportation in cattle results in an increase in heart rate and body temperature 22.

Sheep
Transport compromises the welfare of sheep. Some studies have found experiences associated with loading to be particularly stressful to sheep.

  • There was an increase in the core temperature of sheep after 2.5 hours of road transport 23.
  • Sheep showed heart rate and cortisol increases in response to transport 24,25,26.

4.1.3 The Effects of Transport on Reproduction

Animals in circuses are transported throughout their lives, regardless of age, condition or reproductive status. Transporting animals while pregnant is a cause of suffering and health problems, as discussed in the scientific literature:

  • Pregnant horses transported for nine hours showed signs of prolonged stress associated with abortion or reabsorption of early pregnancies, signaled by a change in the concentration of ascorbic acid in plasma and tissue 27. The study also showed that transportation caused increases in serum progesterone and cortisol, which are also indicators of stress.
  • Thirty mares were tested daily throughout one normal estrous cycle. There was an increase in LH concentrations in transported mares (LH is luteinizing hormone, which regulates the release of progesterone and estrogen) and an increase in cortisol and concentrations of plasma ascorbic acid. The paper concludes that the 12-hour transportation of mares induces “hormonal and plasma ascorbic acid responses indicative of stress” 28.

While it is a common view that horses and other animals become accustomed to transport, we have not seen any scientific evidence of this. Just because an animal undergoes an experience repeatedly does not make the experience less traumatic. It may in fact make the animal more sensitive to the
trauma, and it might be more difficult to identify this.

Many of the scientific papers we have reviewed describe the effects of a single journey, but since circus animals must endure multiple and frequent journeys, at least some of these effects will be multiplied.

4.2 Animal Care Practices and Confinement

Poor animal care practice in the circus may not always be intentional, but it is basically unavoidable, given the mobile nature of circuses and the temporary enclosures, living spaces and trailers that the animals must inhabit.

Even reptiles, which are extremely difficult to care for due to their highly specialized needs such as specific temperature and light requirements, are currently legal in circuses.

The constant moving and the changing environmental conditions cause disruption of normal behavioral patterns, which are likely to leave animals vulnerable to stress and disease. For example, McGreevy14 describes how the timing of sleep is very important in horses. Horses prefer to sleep in the early afternoon, so there should be a minimum of activity at this time. However, in the circus, transport, training and performances do not take this into account.

Enclosures with a restricted amount of space can create a stressful environment for an animal. When animals are housed in groups, the space restrictions can precipitate increased aggression between
individuals, where lower ranking animals cannot escape, and more injuries occur through fighting29.
It is widely accepted by the zoo industry that environmental enrichment the process of improving or enhancing animal environments and care within the context of the animals behavioral biology, i.e., keeping the animals as close as possible to their natural environments30 is essential to the welfare of captive animals.

Environmental enrichment is a dynamic process, requiring speciesspecific modifications, which circuses cannot provide in a meaningful way. The high level of abnormal behavior observed in circus animals testifies to the absence of useful environmental stimulation9.

ADI has videotaped stereotypic behaviors in almost all circus animal species, including horses, ponies, llamas, camels, giraffes, elephants, lions, tigers and bears.

4.2.1 Elephants

Elephants are the largest mammals walking the earth. In the wild they travel up to 13 miles a day, eating a variety of foods, bathing, enjoying a close-knit family life with complex social interactions. As these herds move, they transform the landscape, helping the regeneration of their natural environment.

Elephant Animal Care Practices Bailey Brothers Circus

An ADI field officer worked with Bailey Brothers for two months to gain a picture of the routines for the care of two elephants: Chrissie, a 20-year-old African elephant belonging to Billy Swain, and Boo (or Baby Ruth), a 35-year-old Asian elephant belonging to Mike Swain. Mike Swain presented both elephants during the tour. The animals were on the road for almost 11 months.

Accommodations:
Day:
electric-fenced enclosure (39 ft, 4 ins x 18 ft, 8 ins).
Night: inside trailer (18 ft, 4 ins x 7 ft,10 ins); always chained.
Night (when warm): electric-fenced enclosure, one back foot chained to trailer. This happened only three times during two months of observation. The elephants were chained at least 54% of the time (see below).

Typical Daily Routine
10-10:30a.m: The elephants are unshackled, then let out of their semi/trailer and put in electric-fence enclosure. Each elephant is given around 40-50 gallons of water, having not had access to water for the previous 13 hours (overnight). The elephants are given hay if the enclosure is concrete or allowed to graze if standing on grass.
3:30p.m: Elephants are hosed down and the toenails bleached. They are also washed twice a week, using detergent. Their toenails were filed down once during the two-month period. One of Boo’s was filed so low that it bled. They were body-oiled with vegetable oil once during our observations, to stop the skin from drying out. If they were to be used for elephant rides they would then be dressed (i.e., a rug and saddle were tied to their backs) and put back into the enclosure.
4p.m: They are used for elephant rides for an hour.
5p.m: First show. Chrissie and Boo make a brief appearance.
7:30p.m: Second show. On Saturdays and Sundays the first show starts at 2p.m., with a total of three shows on Saturday and two on Sunday. This program slashed the elephant’s unchained time to just two hours. In addition, whenever Mike Swain left the circus site or was inside his trailer for more than 30 minutes, Chrissie would be chained by a leg to the semi. This would happen every day for two to four hours. This was because she made repeated attempts to escape. This chain was only 21/2 feet long, severely restricting her movements. After each performance the elephants would be fed three scoops of grain each.
9:30p.m: The elephants would be given 30-40 gallons of water, then put into the trailer for the night and chained by a hind leg.

Additional Restriction of Movement
Any additional activity by the circus further restricted these animals’ options to manipulate their own environment. For example, when the circus moved to another location, the elephants were moved out a 6a.m., still in their trailer from the previous night. They remained chained in the trailer for the duration of the journey and then on arrival for another two to three hours while the new site was set up.
Other engagements would result in even more restriction of movement. The elephants once spent a whole day inside the trailer, traveling to a Hindu festival to provide rides. Two days later, they traveled from Austin to Kansas and did not leave the trailer for the entire day.
They had been out of the trailer for only six of the previous 72 hours, and that had been to give rides at the festival.
The next day, the elephants were driven to Butler, Missouri and were not let out of their trailer until noon. Chrissie was immediately chained up outside and was unchained only for the afternoon show.

Behavior
Boo: exhibited abnormal, stereotypic behaviors, including repeated swaying, head bobbing and placing her trunk in her mouth. This usually started around 2p.m., or when left alone in the enclosure while Chrissie gave rides. She also swayed a lot at night in the back of the trailer and during show preparation. When Mike Swain used an aggressive verbal tone Boo placed her trunk in her mouth.
Chrissie: Similar to Boo, she exhibited stereotypic behaviors, including swaying and head bobbing, especially when on her own in the enclosure. When the trailer door was not secured properly during the day she would bang on it with her trunk. She often threw objects (hay, stones, feces, dirt, rubber dishes) at people, both circus workers and members of the public. She would often eat rubbish found in her enclosure, such as plastic and paper bags. When fed by the general public she would snatch the plastic/paper bag containing the food and eat the entire thing. She was occasionally aggressive towards workers and always tried to escape if the electric fence was malfunctioning.
This typical daily routine and the table below, are based on observations made in April and May 2004. This kind of elephant management is similar to practices observed in other circuses, worldwide.

Time spent in different types of activity or accommodations during period of observations:

Chained
Boo: 54%
Chrissie: 66%
Worst days observed for both elephants: 100%

Preparation, performing or rides
Boo: 12%
Chrissie: 12%
Worst days observed for both elephants: 0

Loose in enclosure (grass or concrete)
Boo: 34%
Chrissie: 22%
Worst days observed for both elephants: 0

 

Elephant Accommodations: U.S. examples

Elephants are designed to travel long distances each day, browse a variety of plants, and have a highly developed social structure. The difficulties of containing large, powerful and potentially dangerous animals in temporary or improvised accommodations has a severe impact on their welfare. The circus routine is to chain elephants overnight by a front and hind leg, severely restricting movement to a pace or two backwards and forwards. This period generally lasts from when the animals finish their last performance to the next morning, often 12 hours. During the day, when not performing, giving rides or posing for photos, the most common practice is to enclose the elephants inside a small electric-fenced area. This can be on asphalt, concrete or grass.

UniverSoul Circus, Detroit, Michigan
One African and one Asian elephant, chained by two legs, barely able to move. During two days of observations these elephants were shackled like this when not performing. Both elephants displayed stereotypic behaviors, i.e., abnormal, repetitive, pointless movements. Some experts believe that such behavior indicates that the animal is failing to cope with its environment, it is perhaps trying to relieve the discomfort and frustration of standing in one place for long periods; these repeated movements become a mental habit.

Chains restrict movement for elephants. UniverSoul Circus, Detroit, Michigan
Chains restrict movement for elephants. UniverSoul Circus, Detroit, Michigan

 

Clyde Beatty Cole Brothers Circus, Atlanta, Georgia
Two Asian elephants restricted to a small electric-fenced grass enclosure – barely a couple of elephant paces across. All that was provided was shade, water, hay and access to their trailer.

Clyde Beatty Cole Brothers Circus, Atlanta, Georgia
Clyde Beatty Cole Brothers Circus, Atlanta, Georgia

 

Circus Royale, NewYork State
Nowhere to run, nowhere to hide. African elephants huddled in the center of a tiny electric-fence enclosure, standing on asphalt in a parking lot, with no visible access to water.

Circus Royale, New York State
Circus Royale, New York State

 

Ringling Brothers and Barnum & Bailey Circus, Los Angeles, California
The largest group of Asian elephants we observed: 11 elephants standing in a parking lot with little to occupy their interest and no significant enrichment. There appears to be more space than they are using, but in fact the area is divided by an electric wire. Even with access to the whole area, the space would still be inadequate.

Ringling Brothers and Barnum & Bailey Circus, Los Angeles, California
Ringling Brothers and Barnum & Bailey Circus,
Los Angeles, California

 

Ringling Brothers and Barnum & Bailey Circus, Michigan
Another town, another parking lot or small tent for an elephant to call “home.”

Ringling Brothers and Barnum & Bailey Circus, Michigan
Ringling Brothers and Barnum & Bailey Circus, Michigan

 

Shrine Circus, Minnesota
Three African elephants in a tiny electric-fenced pen during the day only. The elephants stand on grass, but there is no stimulation or access to water. The pen is on the edge of a school playing field with no secondary fencing and occasional attention by circus staff to prevent children from walking up to the animals.

Shrine Circus, Minnesota
Shrine Circus, Minnesota

 

CircusWorld,Wisconsin, annual four day circus festival
Despite being a fixed site, this elephant enclosure for three Asian elephants has similar limitations as those of touring circuses – little space or enrichment. The lower picture shows how quickly an enclosure can become degraded when there is not enough space for elephants to roam.

 

Yet in the circus, these intelligent, social beings are commonly shackled to the ground by one front and one rear leg in order to control them. They can barely take a step or two, forwards and backwards, and cannot exhibit most of their typical behaviors. It also restricts social interactions, because contact is limited to the elephant shackled next to them31. Although many circuses now claim to give their elephants regular access to a pen or outdoor enclosure, the elephants free time is
limited when the circus is constantly on the move, and the elephants have to be prepared for their performances each day. At night, elephants are commonly chained from the time the workers finish their day to when they arrive the next day. This can mean over half the elephants time is spent chained to a stake in the ground or inside a truck9.

Even when elephants are left free in exercise enclosures, these are too small. Consequently, stereotypies occur in captive elephants, regardless of the system of animal care used.

  • One study of circus elephants found that, “Weaving was the most common stereotypic behavior in the elephants, regardless of whether they were picketed [chained] or penned” 32.
  • Another study which saw stereotypic behaviors in all the elephants observed, concluded that “the welfare of closely confined elephants can be poor” 33.
  • High levels of stereotypic and abnormal behaviors were observed in all the elephants involved in the Kirkden and Broom study, which was undertaken in a circus holding facility. This study described how stereotypic behavior differs between individuals and that changing the methods of animal care practices (i.e. shackling versus unshackling), only reduces stereotypies in some individuals. For some elephants it was impossible to identify one underlying cause of stereotypy. Many aspects of the circus environment were found to cause stereotypic behaviors in elephants, such as the lack of social contact, anticipation of food or another significant predictable event, the presence or absence of
  • people, the size of their enclosure, and their proximity to other specific individual elephants. It was also noted that in addition to stereotypies, some circus elephants show other types of abnormal behaviors, such as prolonged inactivity, probably as a result of being confined in an unstimulating environment33.

ADI observation data and videotapes document stereotypic behaviors in all circus animal species we have studied.

4.2.2 Browsing and Grazing Animals

In the wild, browsing and grazing animals spend a large part of their day eating. Circuses cannot provide permanent outdoor paddocks for grazing so these animals spend much of their time confined to indoor stalls or tied to trailers. ADI has found that even when outdoor paddocks are provided, they can be on concrete, asphalt or whatever surface is available. Sites tend to prioritize audience
accessibility and needs rather than cater to animals. This severely restricts the ability of these specialized animals to carry out their natural feeding behaviors.

Browsing and Grazing Animal Care Practices: U.S. examples

Horses, ponies, camels, llamas and similar animals usually spend their days tied up by short ropes or kept in small stalls. We found that only rarely are these animals provided with exercise enclosures. Their only significant exercise is in the ring during rehearsals or performances. Time pressures, including cleaning stalls, watering, brushing and saddling animals up for shows, make it unlikely that these animals can ever enjoy quality free time and space.

Bailey Brothers Circus, Texas
Seven small Shetland ponies and a white horse, Snowy, were generally tied to a six-foot rope attached to a stake or a picket line (running line staked to the ground) without shelter during the day, or kept in a small pen (top).
Between 3 and 4p.m. five of the ponies were tied to the carousel to give rides to children until 9 or 9:30p.m (at least five hours) after which they were tied to the side of a trailer.
Two ponies, Patches and Rebel, were often suspended from this work due to their aggressive behavior, such as bucking. One day, the pony ride attendant reported that the ponies didn’t get enough rest, since they must work the carousel for five continuous hours. One pony had a swollen eye. Another kept trying to get down on her knees. A third had a breathing problem and was hyperventilating and gagging. Snowy had cuts and sores which were attracting flies so a circus worker used Clorox bleach on the open cuts.

 

Clyde Beatty Cole Brothers Circus, Georgia
This is the typical control of performing ponies the world over – on a short tether against the side of a wagon.

Clyde Beatty Cole Brothers Circus, Georgia
Clyde Beatty Cole Brothers Circus, Georgia

 

Circus World, Wisconsin
Below left: Different species share a pen.
Below right: Horses and ponies in circuses get basic stabling, but do not enjoy the benefits of access to pasture provided to equines in other commercial contexts.

 

In the wild, giraffes use their tongues to remove tree leaves and avoid thorns. The absence of this specific challenge in captivity creates a behavioral vacuum and the resultant frustration can cause oral stereotypic behaviors, such as excessive licking, bar biting and tongue playing34. The number of hours an animal is housed indoors can be used to predict the occurrence of stereotypic licking behaviors in browsing and grazing species34. A frustrated feeding motivation may also
result in motion stereotypies, such as pacing34.

Indoor enclosures mean less variety of stimulation than outdoor enclosures, conditions that are very important for reducing the occurrence of stereotypic behaviors in general34.

  • Farmed deer housed indoors during winter exhibit higher levels of aggression, resulting in more injuries to each other, than deer who are confined outdoors in a field. Indoor deer also show an increase in behaviors like chewing each other and their enclosure35.
  • Dama gazelles, particularly the dominant individuals, show more aggression towards their herd-mates when housed in smaller enclosures29.
  • A survey of 257 zoo-housed giraffes and okapis found that almost 80 percent of the animals showed at least one form of stereotypy34.
  • A study of captive black rhinoceroses36 found that they are highly sensitive and respond negatively to the environment and/or social conditions in captivity:
  • Captivity influences the behavior and breeding success of male and female black rhinos differently.
  • Males are affected by a limited enclosure area and by odors, such as a chlorine disinfectant.
  • Female black rhinos are sensitive and react negatively to some aspect of concrete enclosure walls, either the acoustical properties or the visual separation from other black rhinos.
  • A high degree of public accessibility along the perimeter of their enclosures is a potential stressor for both sexes, but especially males. Mortality in captive black rhinos was strongly linked to the percentage of public access along the perimeter of their enclosures.

Another study found that captive black rhinoceroses appear predisposed to a skin disease not found in wild black rhinos. This disease was linked to periods of physical or environmental stress, such as capture, transport, sudden cold or the introduction of a new rhino12. It was also suggested that the unvaried diet of captive rhinos, compared to that of their wild counterparts may play a role in the development of this disease (in the wild rhinos have been observed to browse more than 200 species of plants).

These studies of browsing and grazing animals demonstrate the complexity of captive animals needs and confirm that traveling circuses simply cannot address those needs.

ADI has recorded evidence of aggression in horses during transport and in stable tents. Some animals who are considered difficult or aggressive, like stallions, are not allowed into enclosures and therefore get even less exercise.

4.2.3 Carnivores

The scientific literature shows that carnivores, especially members of the cat family and bears, suffer from captivity. This evidence comes mostly from zoos, where the animals have a permanent residence. Most modern zoos now try to create a habitat as close to the animals natural environment as possible. The temporary nature of the accommodations in traveling circuses makes it impossible to provide the animals with a suitable environment. Therefore, any negative effects of captivity in zoos will be worse in circuses.

Carnivore Accommodations: US examples

For circus lions and tigers, home is usually a cage on the back of a truck – around 8 ft wide by 40 ft long. The average space for each animal is about 61/2 ft by 8 ft, barely larger than the animals themselves, which includes space to defecate. Some circuses provide ‘exercise cages,’ but these are small and usually contain nothing to interest the cats. The animals will get only an hour or two in this space, due to scheduling pressure during the work day.

Clyde Beatty Cole Brothers Circus, Atlanta, Georgia
Seven tigers shared this bare cage on the back of a truck. There was no separate exercise cage, only an extra section of cage bolted onto the end, adding little more than 8 ft x 8 ft. ‘Exercise’ was the time spent in the ring. Not surprisingly, these tigers were pacing and displaying the repetitive movements of severely confined animals.

Shrine Circus, Minnesota
Small wheeled cages like these are typically used to move big cats from their living quarters to the ring to perform. In this instance, the animals were kept in this extremely confined space for an extended period while elephant rides took place during the show’s intermission.

Shrine Circus, Minnesota
Shrine Circus, Minnesota

 

UniverSoul Circus, Detroit, Michigan
The living quarters for tigers on the back of a truck.

UniverSoul Circus, Detroit, Michigan
UniverSoul Circus, Detroit, Michigan

 

Sterling & Reid, Virginia
This three-month-old tiger cub was living with a seven-week-old lion cub and their trainer in his trailer. It is common for circus animals to be taken away from their families, with the trainer replacing the animals’ mothers and siblings. The young tiger was used as a prop for photos during the show, further distorting the animal’s natural behavior.

Sterling & Reid, Virginia
Sterling & Reid, Virginia

 

A study of captive cheetahs37 found that:

  • Captive cheetahs suffer diseases that do not occur in their wild counterparts. These diseases worsen after transport, suggesting an environmental effect.
  • When the cats were moved between facilities for breeding programs, eight of the fifteen animals suffered an increase in corticoid concentration, and six exhibited a prolonged stress response. Of the seven animals who did not experience an increase in corticoid concentration, four had a single peak immediately after movement. The authors concluded that transporting the cheetahs caused the stress response.
  • Corticoid responses increased when the cats were put on public display, and they decreased away from public view, thus indicating a negative welfare response to such exposure 37.

The results of this study are not surprising. Cheetahs in the wild are solitary, have large home ranges, and avoid human contact.

Carnivores frequently show stereotypic behaviors, such as pacing, in captivity. A study38 looked at
nine species of cats in eleven different enclosures at the Edinburgh Zoo. It found that “stereotypic pacing was recorded at various levels in 15 out of 19 cats (79 percent), the levels varying between 1 percent and 32 percent of scans.” The complexity of the animals enclosure was thought to play a big role in the incidence of such behaviors.

A study of leopards39 found that relocating cats to new cages provoked an initial increase in adrenocortical activity and increased hiding behavior. The cats failed to adapt to a new environment.

Captive clouded leopards showed several signs of distress or poor well-being: a high number of stereotypic behaviors, apathy, self-mutilation (e.g., fur plucking and tail chewing), and intersexual aggression (i.e., mate injury and mate killing).

They also achieved poor breeding success and experienced other health problems40.

4.2.4 Primates

Nonhuman primates are our closest relatives. They are highly intelligent and live in close family groups. They suffer in captivity, just as we would. Most nonhuman primate species share more than 90% of their DNA with humans (Homo sapiens)53. The chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) DNA sequence differs from ours by 1-1.5%54, 55. Recent studies have revealed that 99.4 % of the bases are identical within the same genes in humans and chimpanzees. It has therefore been suggested that chimpanzees should take their place alongside humans within the genus Homo56. The primate species vary in size by several orders of magnitude, and are adapted to widely different ways of life mainly in the tropics57. Perhaps the most striking feature of all primates is their high intelligence.

Primate Care and Accommodations: U.S. examples

Primates are highly intelligent, make tools, are arboreal in nature (living mostly in trees) and live in close-knit family groups. Family is important to primates. As their intelligence gives rise to the potential for escape, these animals are severely confined in traveling circuses. They are often separated from their own kind in order to reinforce dependence upon the handler as the only source of food, water, comfort and approval. Primates observed by ADI in circuses are kept in small cages or chained.

Sterling & Reid Circus, Fredericksburg, Virginia
While on public show in the petting zoo, two macaque monkeys were housed individually in cages 6 feet tall by 2 feet wide and deep. This is extremely small for such animals. The cages had limited enrichment, just a few toys and a perch. Overnight the animals were caged elsewhere.

Sterling & Reid Circus, Fredericksburg, Virginia
Sterling & Reid Circus, Fredericksburg, Virginia

 

Bailey Brothers Circus, Texas, Oklahoma, Kansas, Missouri, Illinois
The circus had four macaque monkeys and a solitary capuchin monkey (pictured) which were kept in small cages inside a trailer – even if the entire trailer had been devoted to living space for these primates it is unlikely it could be made adequate. The capuchin lived in complete isolation from its own species and was occasionally moved around with a chain attached around its waist.

Bailey Brothers Circus, Texas, Oklahoma, Kansas, Missouri, Illinois
Bailey Brothers Circus, Texas, Oklahoma, Kansas, Missouri, Illinois

 

Cotton-top tamarins can solve tasks that require the use of one object to attain another, for example the use of a tool to obtain food. Without training, they can discriminate between tools of different colour, texture, shape and material, in terms of their ability to retrieve food58. Wild chimpanzees have been observed creating tools in order to feed on termites59. Rhesus macaques can perform rudimentary arithmetic and think using symbols60. Considered paramount among human cognitive abilities is the capacity to reason about what others think, want and see; a capacity referred to as
theory of mind.

Rhesus macaques have been shown to possess an essential component of theory of mind the ability to deduce what others perceive on the basis of where they are looking61. Chimpanzees show cross modal transfer of information in that they can, for example, recognize by touch alone objects that they have previously only seen, and can understand and use abstract symbols in their communication62. Humans and chimpanzees differ in intellectual ability, but what differs is their degree of intelligence, not the kind of mental processes employed. There is no bold line separating human intelligence from chimpanzee intelligence60. Chimpanzees have even learned and then taught other chimpanzees American sign language (Fouts).

Captivity affects different animal species in very different ways. There is however consensus among researchers regarding the higher risks of suffering for the species with more intelligence and cognitive abilities63. It is clear that the impoverished environment and lack of stimulation in the traveling circus seriously compromises the welfare of these animals.

These are species for whom social contact with their own kind is a vital component of any environment. As primates ourselves it should be easy for us to comprehend the impact of social isolation or limited social contact on these species, since their social behaviors so often reflect elements of our own.

Chimpanzees display a range of postures and gestures similar to those of humans in similar situations. They greet with kisses and embraces, they play and reassure each other and express emotions including anger and sadness, they seek reassurance and comfort like humans. They hold hands, pat each other on the back, swagger, and shake their fists, tickle and laugh. Not only that, but they do so in the same kind of context as ourselves62.

There are similarities amongst other primates. For example during courtship capuchin monkeys use many of the same behaviors that humans do, such as gazing into each others eyes, tilting their heads, raising their foreheads and blinking their eyes64.We primates are social. The macaque social system is based on specific rules for relationships and socialn behavior and rhesus macaques develop long-lasting social bonds that may endure a lifetime65. Capuchinmonkeys work together to gather food and then share the fruits of their labour, the kind of cooperation essential to human society60. We
nurture and teach our children over a long period. Peer play is important in the development of young rhesus macaque monkeys because it promotes social development. During play, chimpanzees learn about nonverbal and other forms of communication66. Juvenile vervet monkeys display exploratory behavior (such as playing with a new toy) which is common to both vervets and humans53.

Nonhuman primates have been found to be susceptible to various forms of stress, which might cause changes in behavior (e.g. social behavior, stereotypic behavior) and hormone concentrations (e.g. the corticosteroid level)67. Stress may be psychological as well as physical68. There is a clear danger of circus primates adopting stereotypic and abnormal behavior. Such behavior among primates has been observed in zoos, and has been attributed to a variety of factors69. For chimpanzees, socially deprived individuals (e.g. those that were separated from their mothers at an early age) show reduced levels of normal behaviors and a higher level of abnormal behavior, as well as a wider range of abnormal behaviors70.

A study into stereotypic behavior among primates in captivity found that the animals with the most marked stereotypic behavior were those that had been hand-reared by humans. The authors also found that animals that had been confiscated from touring zoos, circuses and animal traders exhibited higher levels of abnormal behaviors than did animals reared in larger, recognized zoos71. Hand rearing is popular amongst most performing animal establishments because it teaches reliance and compliance at an early age, and infant animals are popular with audiences and for photo opportunities. Logically there will be negative behavioral impacts from continually removing young animals from their mothers.

The demands of continual transport and readjustment appear to have been directly related to abnormal primate behavior, which itself usually indicates that the animal’s psychological welfare is at a suboptimal level69.

Performing animal handlers require regular one-to-one contact with young primates to maintain control, dependence and obedience. The handler becomes the only source of food, water, and approval. Thus, trainers deprive performing primates of normal social contact with their own kind, locking them into a lonely world where food, water, and affection are handed out as a prize for compliance. The chimpanzees smile which you so often see is, in reality,a grimace of fear. It is a sad irony that it is our empathy for these creatures that is being exploited by the cynical trainer.

4.2.5 Domestic Animals

Circuses include many domestic animals, such as common pet parakeets (budgerigars), cats, dogs and horses, in their acts. Although there are few scientific studies on household pets that would apply to the circus situation, these animals are subject to many of the same problems as exotic species.

Indicators of poor welfare may be less obvious in these domesticated animals than in captive wild animals, but evidence shows that they do suffer from poor animal care and confinement:

  • Dogs have an inherent desire for social contact and are not suited to being confined alone in cages or kennels. Housing dogs alone results in boredom, under-stimulation and behavior problems. Visual and tactile contact with other dogs improves a dogs psychological well-being and alters negative behavior patterns41.
  • Both domestic horses and captive exotic equids, such as zebras and Przewalski horses (native to Mongolia), show a variety of stereotypic behaviors when stabled or confined, including, crib-biting, wind-sucking, wood-chewing, weaving, pawing, trailer and door-kicking, self-biting and head tossing14.
  • Domestic cats may show increased hiding behaviors as a response to stimuli or changes in their environment and to avoid interactions with other cats or people. They are more likely to respond to poor environmental conditions by becoming inactive and by inhibiting normal behaviors, such as self-maintenance (feeding, grooming and elimination), exploration, or play, than by exhibiting abnormal behavior42.

To summarize, the fact that an animal is of a domesticated species does not mean that it will suffer less when confined for long periods of time to a small space or in a box. Broom & Johnson: “…neither early training nor genetic selection can push the individual beyond its biological potential and a profound lack of stimulation is something to which no vertebrate animal is likely to be able to adapt” 4.

4.3 Inappropriate Social Groupings and Isolation

It is vitally important to house species in appropriate groups. For many species social living provides more benefits than finding food or avoiding predation. It is a major source of stimulation and life enrichment. The social environment of many species represents a constant source of mental stimulation, the complexity and variety of which animal keepers cannot replace by any form of artificial environmental enrichment30.

Changing social groups and dynamics and removing an animal for training, performance or transport can lead to periods of social isolation or can bring animals into contact with new groups. Often, individual animals are exchanged between circuses or lent to another circus for a season, resulting in long-term disruption of social groups.

Social species, such as elephants, are often kept in isolation, while animals who are naturally solitary, such as tigers, are commonly kept in groups6.

4.3.1 Social Isolation and Separation from Companions

Animals in circuses can experience different types of separation separation from their own kind (especially problematic for social species) and separation from companions who may or may not be of the same species.

The detrimental effects of social deprivation and separation have been widely documented in many species and are known to cause behavioral and physiological indications of stress43.

  • In a study of social separation in giraffes, a male was removed from his two female companions. The females displayed protest behaviors, including increased activity, stereotypical behaviors, and increased contact with each other. They also showed decreased habitat utilization. These results were consistent with the findings of studies of other species, particularly nonhuman primates, where the first change in behavior is protest, characterized by increased vocalization, motion and stereotypical behavior, as well contact and clinging if the animal is housed with peers during the separation. Giraffes are not known to be highly social animals in the wild, yet their aversion to this kind of social separation proves that a complex social structure is not a prerequisite for social attachment43.
  • Spectacled bears have been documented displaying stereotypic repetitive head-tossing as a result of social frustration when they were prevented from interacting with other bears who were in close proximity to them44.
  • In the wild, elephants live in groups and display complex social behavior. The natural grouping of both African and Asian elephants is of a family unit, and the social bonds among the members of the family are very strong45. Such family units are not possible in circuses, where elephant groupings are varied and changeable. Supporting this, a study on circus elephants found that the limited opportunity for social contact was the principle factor in the female Asian elephants stereotypy33.
  • Piglets isolated from others showed increased sitting, pawing behavior and escape attempts and decreased activity, play and interest in novel objects. All these changes reflected negative impacts on the piglets welfare46.
  • Social separation in cattle induced struggling and large increases in vocalization, heart rate and plasma cortisol concentrations. The mere presence of other cattle was sufficient to prevent struggling and vocalization, regardless of peer identity, which shows that nonspecific attachments can develop between individual cows and their peers47.

4.3.2 Forced Proximity to Other Animals

Tigers in the wild are solitary animals, coming together only for mating. A study on circus tigers described how circuses often transport tigers in groups and that severe fights can break out6. Major movement in the back of a truck could be felt by a driver, indicating fighting. Whenever this happened, the method of stopping the fight was the sudden application of the brakes, which would cause the fight to stop until the truck could be stopped, and a handler could separate the tigers. Clearly an unacceptable situation.

Traveling circuses frequently put different species of large cats together and have even created a lion-tiger hybrid, calling it a “liger.” Putting different species of large cats together can lead to serious aggression, as when a male tiger attacked a lioness, causing very serious injuries.

Studies of browsing and grazing species have shown that increased social density may produce competition for resources, especially food, which could then increase the likelihood of stereotypic licking34.

Circus Animal Control Methods

One of the great circus myths is that circus animals are trained by kindness and reward, also known as positive reinforcement. Our studies of the training of animals in circuses over the past 15 years have shown that the tools of the trade are whips, goads, iron bars, elephant hooks and electric shock devices.

 

The actual training takes place in secret. The ‘training sessions’ presented to public and media when on the road, are in fact just rehearsals, when the animals are simply being put through well-worn paces.
Circus animals are shown during these rehearsals, that if they disobey the trainer, they will be punished. Even huge, powerful animals can be beaten into submission.
Young animals, so inquisitive and playful, will grow old in the same barren cages, and they will learn from an early age, that disobedience will not be tolerated. It is a lesson that will be repeated throughout their lives.
The elephant hook or ankus is common to the traveling circus around the globe.

 

4.3.3 Inappropriate Groupings with Other Species

Besides suffering from the effects of isolation, animals suffer when forced to live in close proximity to other species, as is often the case in circuses. The suffering is further increased when the other species is a natural predator or prey of the animal in question, as documented in ADI observations.

When next to cattle or pigs, farmed red deer stayed as far as possible from them and were generally more active, showed agonistic interactions and had elevated plasma cortisol concentrations. Despite previous familiarization of some of the deer with cattle, the more experienced animals did not habituate to the unusual grouping. In fact, evidence suggested that previous exposure to cattle made red deer more aversive to them48.

Like other browsing and grazing species, deer have evolved a naturally exaggerated flight distance as an adaptation for escape from predators. In a confined space, where they are housed close to other species, they cannot prevent these other animals from entering their flight zone.

Obviously, ruminants and omnivores like cows and pigs do not pose a predatory threat to deer, but they may pose other threats. Firstly, they are not familiar to deer and are thus perceived as a potential threat. Secondly, they may be competition for resources. Thirdly, different species have evolved specialized means of communicating with one another, and the unpredictable noises from another species and an inability to read each others signals may cause problems in mixed species groups48.

4.3.4 Close Proximity of Predator and Prey Species

Prey species have adapted to recognize, avoid and defend against predators49. However, in the restrictions of a traveling circus, animals such as horses, other browsing and grazing animals and smaller animals are frequently exposed to one or more predators, such as dogs, tigers, lions and bears.

Predators may also become stressed and frustrated by the presence of prey species they are unable to hunt or of competing predators, with whom they cannot compete.

  • Studies of mammalian changes in behavior when exposed to predators have shown responses such as anxiety and long-lasting neural circuit changes in the brain50.
  • For many mammalian species, predator avoidance relies on sensitivity to predator odors. Research on the effect of predator odors in mammalian prey species describes how pregnant female rodents exposed to such odors may give birth to smaller litters and exposure to such odors in early life can also hinder normal development. Studies of mammalian species who avoid predator odors includes rodents, possums and sheep49.
  • Behavioral effects in animals exposed to predator odors include inhibited activity, suppression of nondefensive behaviors, such as foraging, feeding and grooming, and shifts to habitats or secure locations where such odors are not present49.
  • Even closely related animals, such as different species of exotic cats, can be averse to the presence of other species. A zoo study investigating the low reproductive success rate of small exotic cats in captivity pointed out that most of
  • these cats are solitary in nature. However, in captivity they are routinely housed in pairs and managed in close proximity to other species of small and larger cats, whom they may perceive as potential predators51 or competitors for resources and territory. Cats try to avoid contact with other cats and show abnormal and stress behaviors when other cats encroach on their territory. This was cited as a possible reason for the lack of breeding success with this species in captivity.

Public Safety Issues

When animals are kept in temporary cages and tents, safety is compromised. Circus workers and members of the public, including children, have been killed and maimed by circus animals – lions, tigers and elephants have all escaped.
Many circus working practices increase the likelihood of such incidents by bringing people into dangerously close proximity to wild animals. They may display animals in inappropriate, uncontrolled areas to attract publicity or to take advantage of a venue that is not suited for the exhibition of wild animals.
Any animal can be unpredictable, especially when stressed or if it sees an opportunity to escape its confinement. The 2007 San Francisco Zoo tragedy illustrated how agile big cats can escape even a purpose-built facility. It is easy to see that the risks are much greater in the portable facilities described here.

A. This lion at the permanent quarters of Chipperfield Enterprises in the U.K. (at the time a supplier of acts to the U.S.) has climbed onto a prop and is attempting to escape through a hole in the netting of a training ring. This illustrates how animals can exploit deficiencies in containment, leading to dangerous situations.
A. This lion at the permanent quarters of Chipperfield Enterprises in the U.K. (at the time a supplier of acts to the U.S.) has climbed onto a prop and is attempting to escape through a hole in the netting of a training ring. This illustrates how animals can exploit deficiencies in containment, leading to dangerous situations.

 

B & C:  Shrine Circus, Minnesota Tigers wait to perform – there is no netting over the ring cage.  Many question the safety of elephant rides, yet an elephant carrying children is walked next to the caged tigers, where there is an increased risk of the elephant being spooked.
B & C: Shrine Circus, Minnesota
Tigers wait to perform – there is no netting over the ring cage. Many question the safety of elephant rides, yet an elephant carrying children is walked next to the caged tigers, where there is an increased risk of the elephant being spooked.
D:  Bailey Brothers Circus, Oklahoma and Kansas  Chrissie the elephant escaped frequently, even dismantling the electric fence; she threw hay, grass and stones at people and had a reputation for cornering and pushing circus workers.  Despite this potentially dangerous behavior, the circus allowed Chrissie to be fed by the public, separated by just a small metal barrier. April 17: Chrissie escaped by lifting up the electric wire (which was turned on) and walking underneath it.  Several workers ran after her with elephant hooks. April 19: Chrissie tried to escape and threw grass and hay at people during the day. April 20: Chrissie escaped, so was chained up for most of the day.  She threw hay, stones and grass at people. April 22: Chrissie tried to escape by dismantling the electric fence.  Her trainer said that he thought she was becoming accustomed to the shock.
D: Bailey Brothers Circus, Oklahoma and Kansas
Chrissie the elephant escaped frequently, even dismantling the electric fence; she threw hay, grass and stones at people and had a reputation for cornering and pushing circus workers. Despite this potentially dangerous behavior, the circus allowed Chrissie to be fed by the public, separated by just a small metal barrier.
April 17: Chrissie escaped by lifting up the electric wire (which was turned on) and walking underneath it. Several workers ran after her with elephant hooks.
April 19: Chrissie tried to escape and threw grass and hay at people during the day.
April 20: Chrissie escaped, so was chained up for most of the day. She threw hay, stones and grass at people.
April 22: Chrissie tried to escape by dismantling the electric fence. Her trainer said that he thought she was becoming accustomed to the shock.

 

E:  Circus Royale, Glens Falls, New York State Twice a day, three elephants had to walk from their living quarters (1) to the performance venue (2) and then back again along a public road.
E: Circus Royale, Glens Falls, New York State
Twice a day, three elephants had to walk from their living quarters (1) to the performance venue (2) and then back again along a public road.

 

F:  Bailey Brothers Circus, Illinois Penny the zebra escaped twice from her pen in four days.  She would put her head through the metal railings and lift the hinges to escape.  She also barged the railings and paced when her companion pony, Tony, was away performing.  Penny would also try to escape when workers moved her from the enclosure to her trailer, pulling and kicking.  Occasionally, she bit circus workers and members of the public.
F: Bailey Brothers Circus, Illinois
Penny the zebra escaped twice from her pen in four days. She would put her head through the metal railings and lift the hinges to escape. She also barged the railings and paced when her companion pony, Tony, was away performing. Penny would also try to escape when workers moved her from the enclosure to her trailer, pulling and kicking. Occasionally, she bit circus workers and members of the public.

 

Unnatural Acts: U.S. examples

Circus animal acts do not teach respect for animals or appreciation of the other species who share our planet. In fact, they teach the opposite. Audiences are shown a caricature of an animal, often presented to make the trainer look strong and brave. While circuses strive for spectacle, animals are forced into increasingly bizarre and unnatural acts that they would naturally resist. So force is used to make them obey.

 

Acts appearing in the U.S. during the study include tigers forced to jump through rings of fire, elephants doing front leg stands and a boxing kangaroo. Even domestic cats are pushed to their limit, leaping from a great height to land on a cushion.

 

4.4. Conclusions on the Scientific Evidence on Suffering

All together, this evidence demonstrates that whether of exotic or domestic species, animals are likely to suffer as a result of living in a traveling circus:

  • Transport has been shown to cause many indicators of stress, including increased heart rate, a rise in body temperature, lowered immunity to illness and disease, changes in hormone levels that are known to affect pregnancies, weight loss, increased instances of aggression and stereotypic behaviors.
  • Inadequate animal care practices and limitations of space make it impossible for animals to express normal behavior. This leads to a high number of stereotypies and other abnormal behaviors, increased aggression towards other animals, increased susceptibility to disease, the presence of physiological indicators of stress and greater mortality.
  • Inappropriate social groupings cause a multitude of negative effects on animals:
  • Isolation or separation from companions leads to complex changes in behavior, including a decreased interest in surroundings, stereotypies, increased heart rate, vocalizations and higher levels of physiological stress.
  • Animals forced to live in close proximity with one another show a greater frequency of fighting and competitive behaviors and a greater incidence of stereotypies.
  • When different species are mixed or forced to live in close proximity to one another they exhibit a range of avoidance behaviors and increases in heart rate and other indicators of physiological stress and spend more time being alert.
  • When predators are in close proximity to prey, the prey shows anxiety behaviors, changes in their nervous systems, a suppression of feeding and grooming behaviors and often a lowered breeding success. When they do breed, the presence of a predators odor can lead to smaller litter sizes and can hinder the normal development of young.

The horse data helps us to assess the effects of constant confinement, transport and performing on a whole range of other animals. Horses have a long-established relationship with humans, but there is clear evidence of the suffering of horses in all aspects of circus life. How much more, then, must other species, especially those instinctively afraid of humans, suffer?

Given the circumstances, it is simply not possible for traveling circuses to provide animals with the space and environment they need to maintain optimum physical and psychological health.

The abnormal behaviors seen in animals in the U.S. justify a call for an end to the use of animals in traveling circuses. If there is doubt about the identification of injury or poor animal welfare, the animals should be given the benefit of the doubt.

Read the Executive Summary of the Report.

1 Introduction
2 The Traveling Environment
3 Pilot Study: Animals in Traveling Circuses in the U.S.
4 The Scientific Evidence
5 The Animal Welfare Act
6 Recommendations for Action
7 Appendix: Public Opinion
8 References

Comments

  1. Elliot Michaels · 14 July 2008 at 9:00a

    At what point will we reach a level of decency, humanity and compassion. The United states is far behind other countries such as the UK which has banned animal acts in circuses.
    I hope that I will witness the termination of atrocities against animals in my lifetime.

Leave a comment on this story

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *

This site uses Akismet to reduce spam. Learn how your comment data is processed.